Category Archives: Scientific Encounters

Wandering Thoughts and the Future

Thoughts exist within a world of references and intentions. Content analysis of “wandering” thoughts has shown that such “stimulus-independent” thoughts are largely goal-directed and future oriented (Baird et al, 2011). Thoughts can be conversational and goal-directed at the same time. Imaginary conversations are like rehearsals; we silently speak to ourselves when planning. Even when we are rehashing past conversations, what may start as an act of memory often becomes elaborated, with what we shoulda, coulda, woulda said, if only we had thought of it at the time. This is one way the social animal prepares for future interactions.

Reference:

Benjamin Baird , Jonathan Smallwood, and Jonathan W. Schooler (2011) Back to the future: Autobiographical planning and the functionality of mind-wandering. Consciousness and Cognition Volume 20, Issue 4, 604–1611

Irrational Beliefs, Or Are They?

Here I am thinking about the type of beliefs much discussed in  clinical psychology,  such as the following “irrational” beliefs  identified by Albert Ellis:

  • It is a dire necessity for adult humans to be loved or approved by virtually every significant other person in their community.
  • One absolutely must be competent, adequate and achieving in all important respects or else one is an inadequate, worthless person.
  • People absolutely must act considerately and fairly and they are damnable villains if they do not. They are their bad acts.
  • It is awful and terrible when things are not the way one would very much like them to be.
  • Emotional disturbance is mainly externally caused and people have little or no ability to increase or decrease their dysfunctional feelings and behaviors.
  • If something is or may be dangerous or fearsome, then one should be constantly and excessively concerned about it and should keep dwelling on the possibility of it occurring.
  • One cannot and must not face life’s responsibilities and difficulties and it is easier to avoid them.
  • One must be quite dependent on others and need them and you cannot mainly run one’s own life.
  • One’s past history is an all-important determiner of one’s present behavior and because something once strongly affected one’s life, it should indefinitely have a similar effect.
  • Other people’s disturbances are horrible and one must feel upset about them.
  • There is invariably a right, precise and perfect solution to human problems and it is awful if this perfect solution is not found.

These “beliefs” are not believable as beliefs. I doubt the Ellis list of irrational beliefs would ever be spontaneously provided in response to an open-ended question.  Sure, a lot of us may endorse weak versions, like “I have a hard time meeting my responsibilities and often avoid them” – but then weak versions cease to be irrational – they’re simply proclivities. Ellis uses the absolutist, hyperbolic and unforgiving language of 19th century preachers to create strawman beliefs that are obviously irrational but which no one holds.

All sorts of beliefs  may be articulated when queried* but few beliefs exist sentence-like in the head, or exist at all  before being elicited in some way.  Beliefs in general are emergent phenomena – that is, they emerge from the interactions of multiple and distributed micro-events in the brain, becoming a coherent thing only when presented to self and/or other. They emerge when something brings them out: a question or questionnaire/ an imaginary or real act of communication. Such beliefs emerge as they are put into words.

We put things into words to communicate: to ourselves or others, in our heads or in the world. Communication is a form of behavior: it is doing something to achieve a goal. So when someone says something obviously exaggerated and irrational, how much of that is expressing a true belief and how much is a behavior?  When I’m furious at a guy and complain to a friend that “all men are assholes”, do I truly believe that?

Consider such venting as a behavior:  what I am trying to accomplish with the behavior? What’s the pay-off? Perhaps an attempt to become reconciled to being wronged, or to feel the exhilaration of righteous indignation, or to exhaust the feeling through excess or to solicit commiseration? And even if I believe the sentiment in the heat of the moment, do I truly believe it?  Is it a transient belief, just passing through? Or is it a sleeping-dog belief that is quiescent most of the time, but comes to the fore with the right trigger? (But always “there”, deep-down).

Reference

Ellis, A. (1994). Reason and Emotion in Psychotherapy, NY: Birch Lane Press

Anxiety, Fear, and the Comfort of Attachment

In the last post I said that anxiety and fear are more responses to the absence of comforting beliefs than the presence of uncomfortable beliefs.

It’s like with attachment theory, where so much hinges on whether the toddler perceives the caregiver as a safe haven. A toddler that lacks such faith in the caregiver copes poorly with stress and uncertainty. A toddler who is securely attached to the caregiver is freer to explore new environments, confident there will be a secure base to return to.

Secure attachment provides a foundation for resilience and a sense of control. A secure toddler can always return to home base, where she will be safe and loved. Or at least she will  have faith that the caregiver will return. That certainty encourages exploratory behavior – going where baby has not gone before – which in turn increases tolerance of uncertainty and a willingness to power through anxiety….resulting in a succession of discoveries and delights, setting the stage for more confident explorations.

Of course, early experience is not destiny.

Reference:

Ainsworth,  MD (December 1969). “Object relations, dependency, and attachment: a theoretical review of the infant-mother relationship”. Child Development. Blackwell Publishing. 40 (4): 969–1025. doi:10.2307/1127008

Anxiety, Fear and Beliefs

Anxiety and fear are fed more by the absence of belief than its presence. Cognitive psychologists often see psychological dysfunction as a matter of “irrational beliefs”. And they point to evidence of such beliefs from self-report questionnaires that consist of forced-choice questions: do you believe ‘x’ or not? Problem is, the essence of “belief” is faith – certainty.   Yet certainty is often a matter of degree:  it’s not either you have it or you don’t – it’s how much of it do you have.

If an opinion is less than certain, it is not a belief.

What lets fear in is the uncertainty, not the belief.

Uncertainty without the compensation of belief – that ultimately it will work out, that there is a secure harbor, despite the presence confusion – creates a vacuum that is filled by alarm.

“Bad things will happen” is not the language of fear. “Bad things might happen” is.

 

Self-Regulating for Pleasure

“Self-regulation is defined here as the process of purposefully directing one’s actions, thoughts, and feelings toward a goal (Carver & Scheier, 2011). A goal is a cognitive construct that specifies an intended outcome, typically one that is relatively long in duration and wide in scope compared to immediate or hedonic goals. For example, “quit smoking” is a goal that might require self-regulation for a smoker because it would compete or conflict with the immediate goal to “smoke a cigarette”. My working definition, then, is that self-regulation is the capacity to enact psychologically distant goals in favor of psychologically proximal ones.”  -Berkman (2016)

I disagree with Berkman. Thinking about my own struggles with self-regulation, the struggle is only sometimes about Bigger goals (e.g., long-term or broader) competing with Smaller goals (e.g., “immediate or hedonic” goals). Often the struggle is between (or among!) goals of similar “levels”. Like whether to say or not what I believe to be true. One side of the struggle may play the role of the “temptation” (just say it!) and the other side is advising caution (she doesn’t care, so what’s the point?).  More sides may chime in (in the long-term, she’ll benefit, even in the short-term she’ll be dismissive; or, if you say it enough, it will eventually make an impression; or, it really doesn’t matter – it is pride that insists on truth-telling and humility is a virtue).

Berkman also seems to assume that long-term or broader goals are better in some way. But often the struggle is when to give in and when not to – when to yield to the immediate pleasure or impulse and when to resist.  When restraint is the default, giving in can be effortful. Living a life of pleasure can be a “broad goal” with deep philosophic roots. (Check out Epicurus on that score).

There is a time to give into temptations and a time to resist them. Whenever there’s a tug-of-war among competing goals, and you have to override one behavior or goal in favor of another, self-regulation is involved. Enjoying what the moment has to offer is a worthy goal. When to honor that goal is the question.

Reference:

Berkman, Elliot T. Self-regulation training. To appear in K. D. Vohs & R. F. Baumeister (Eds.), Handbook of Self-Regulation: Research,Theory and Applications (3rd Edition). New York: Guilford.

 

Self-Regulation Isn’t Just About Spoiling the Fun

Self-regulation is a internal goal management process where we override or preempt one goal in favor of another. By ‘goal’ I mean an outcome and the forces marshaled by that outcome: behaviors, emotions, and attention. Don’t do that, calm down, look the other way, think of something else.

Self-regulation isn’t all negative. It’s not all about inhibiting one thing to allow another.

Self-regulation can also be a booster of energy, of motivation, and of action.

Self-regulation is more than “Don’t do it!” It’s also “Just do it!”

(Of course, self-regulation doesn’t always have to be an exclamation-mark experience).

References:

Hofmann, Wilhelm; Baumeister, Roy F.; Förster, Georg; Vohs, Kathleen D. Everyday temptations: An experience sampling study of desire, conflict, and self-control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Vol 102(6), Jun 2012, 1318-1335. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0026545

Inzlicht, M., Legault, L. and  Teper , R. Exploring the Mechanisms of Self-Control Improvement Current Directions in Psychological Science August 2014 vol. 23 no. 4 302-307

Self-Regulate: It’s What We Do

Self-regulation is often defined as a homeostatic process: you’ve got the set point (goal, standard, value, or ideal); you detect a discrepancy in your “system” (e.g., goal-incongruent behavior, goal-undermining internal state – like feeling rage when you’re trying to be nice); and then you take corrective action (e.g., shut the fuck up, take a deep breath, walk away).  Just like how a thermostat works.

According to one time-sampling study, we are self-regulating about half our waking hours. Usually everything goes smoothly, the process is pretty automatic, we rarely notice, or at least rarely remember (unless beeped by some researcher right after we have righted ourselves). Most of the time, we’re successful at self-regulating – back on track, no problem. Temptation resisted. Angry words suppressed. Attention redirected to the lecture.

Self-regulation doesn’t have to be reactive only. We can proactively self-regulate to avert potential dysregulation. Eat a snack before grocery shopping.  Practice yoga. Or do a little “mindful” breathing before walking into that meeting. Relaxed and ready for the challenge.

References:

Hofmann, Wilhelm; Baumeister, Roy F.; Förster, Georg; Vohs, Kathleen D. Everyday temptations: An experience sampling study of desire, conflict, and self-control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Vol 102(6), Jun 2012, 1318-1335. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0026545

Inzlicht, M., Legault, L. and  Teper , R. Exploring the Mechanisms of Self-Control Improvement Current Directions in Psychological Science August 2014 vol. 23 no. 4 302-307

Want to Convert a Climate Change Skeptic? Some Basics Rules of Thumb

Basic Rule of Thumb #1: if the person you are trying to persuade doesn’t like or trust you, continuing to insist that catastrophic climate change will definitely happen without major self-sacrifice, when that can’t be known for sure, basically erodes trust even further. By “you”, I mean the category to which you have been assigned.  For many climate change skeptics, that would be “environmental activist” or “climate change alarmist”.

Basic Rule of Thumb #2: don’t assume all climate change skeptics are the same. According to various surveys, few skeptics are “strong skeptics” – those convinced it’s all a bunch of hogwash and don’t care what the “consensus” is. More are “lukewarmers” or moderate skeptics, who take exception to some part of the message: how much the climate is likely to change, various  inadequacies of climate change models, or what needs to be done to mitigate and/or adapt to climate change.

Basic Rule of Thumb #3: don’t assume climate change skeptics are simply ignorant. Skeptics are about  as knowledgeable as those who accept the “consensus”. Interestingly, many skeptics report they used to be more concerned about climate change but became skeptical upon further study.

Basic Rule of Thumb #4: just because strong emotions are involved doesn’t mean there isn’t a valid point behind the emotions. Try to understand and respect that point. You can still stand your ground.

References:

Bashir, N. Y., Lockwood, P., Chasteen, A. L., Nadolny, D., & Noyes, I. (2013). The ironic impact of activists: Negative stereotypes reduce social change influence. European Journal of Social Psychology, 43, 614–626. doi:10.1002/ejsp.1983 [CrossRef], [Web of Science ®]

Matthews, Paul (2015) Why Are People Skeptical about Climate Change? Some Insights from Blog Comments, Environmental Communication , 9:2, 153-168, DOI: 10.1080/17524032.2014.999694 To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17524032.2014.999694

Election 2016: Chapter 2: Climate Change and Energy Issues  (“Views about climate change are roughly the same regardless of level of science knowledge. There are no differences in views between those with a degree in a scientific field and those with training in other fields.” )  http://www.pewinternet.org/2015/07/01/chapter-2-climate-change-and-energy-issues/

Hyperbolic Discounting and Climate Change

Consequences which occur at a later time, good or bad, tend to have a lot less bearing on our choices the more distantly they fall in the future… even when one’s life is at stake. “

–  Alan Bellows  https://www.damninteresting.com/hyperbolic-discounting/

The immediate future looms large in human psychology. People tend to care more about near-term payoff or danger than what might be coming down the pike in a few years. This tendency to downplay later rewards or threats – called hyperbolic discounting – probably evolved because  prehistoric conditions were too harsh for long-term calculations to be of much benefit.  Live for today because tomorrow may never come.

The bigger the delay, the greater the uncertainty, the less likely a person will care enough to do much about it.  Insisting that something bad will happen years from now doesn’t increase certainty or urgency to act, especially if the argument relies on highly complex models and debatable assumptions.  Nor does an argument from authority persuade if one doesn’t respect or trust that authority.

“Think for yourself” and “question authority” are cultural norms – inconsistently observed yet still valued. Push against these norms and you get resistance. Push harder and you get more resistance. Repeat over and over, and they just walk away.

Think like a scientist, Act like a Doctor

Think like a scientist, act like a doctor. That’s how I try to approach politics and economics. It’s my standard, by which I measure the good and the true and which provides a point of reference for self-correction.

Without ideals, there is no progress towards the good and the true. Of course, ideals are also a source of great harm and suffering, what with their affinity for the bad and the false. But, then, to truly think like a scientist counteracts the excesses of idealism.

The dark side of idealism is often the result of arrogance and certainty: I know what will fix the world. I have the answer. It’s self-evident. It’s obvious. Only fools and villains think otherwise.

In contrast, the spirit of science is that of humility. You don’t know for sure. You could be wrong.. What seems to be the case may not be the case at all. And yet the scientific project is predicated on hope: you can get closer to the truth through careful application of the Method: come up with testable hypotheses, make sure they’re falsifiable, and systematically test each one. Be transparent about your data and procedures, so others can critique your approach or try to replicate your findings.

Like scientists, medical doctors appreciate their own limitations. Yet they are tasked with making important decisions – possibly life-and-death decisions – despite not knowing for sure they’ve got it right.  Wait and see? Try something? Try something else?  All the while observing and thinking and investigating further. Doctors need to be willing to act boldly, willing to do nothing, and willing to change their minds. Because the health of the patient is what’s important – not a foolish consistency with past opinions.

So it should be when the body politic is the patient.